Researchers have long pondered over the reason for zebra’s stripes.
Over the past decade, Professor Tim Caro at the
University of Bristol’s School of Biological Sciences has examined
and discredited many popular theories such as their use as camouflage from
predators, a cooling mechanism through the formation of convection currents and
a role in social interactions.
Stripes acting to confuse predators is another common
explanation, but it too is flawed when looking at the scientific data. Instead,
mounting evidence suggests that it is parasitic flies that are confounded by
the zebra’s distinctive patterning.
In a new paper published in the journal Proceedings of
the Royal Society B, the Bristol scientists have now provided significant depth
to this hypothesis by narrowing down the possible mechanism.
Previously, the same researchers had shown that
blood-sucking horseflies would approach horses in striped rugs as often as
plain rugs, but then fail to land or slow down when they got close.
It seems the stripes dazzled the flies, forcing them to collide with the skin or fly away
altogether. In their new study, the research team explored a potential
mechanism explaining how stripes could lead to this outcome: the “aperture
effect.”
Lead author Dr Martin How, also from Bristol’s School
of Biological Sciences, explains: “The aperture effect is a well-known optical
illusion that, in human vision, is also known as the barber-pole effect. Moving
stripes, such as those on the rotating barber-pole signs outside barbershops,
appear to move at right angles to the stripe, rather than in their true
direction, so the pole appears to move upwards, rather than around its axle.
“We set out to see if this illusion also takes place in the
eyes of biting flies as they come to land on striped hosts.
“As any fly approaches a landing surface, it will adjust its
speed according to how quickly the surface expands across its vision, enabling
a slowed and controlled landing.
“Stripes however could disrupt this ‘optic flow’ through the
aperture effect, leading the fly to believe the landing surface is further away
than reality. Thus, the fly fails to slow down or land successfully.”
This latest study assessed flies landing on horses wearing
striped, checked, or plain grey rugs. The researchers found that flies hardly
landed on checked or striped rugs at all.
The researchers explain that, since checked rugs provide
visual input free from the aperture effect, one would have expected flies to
land on them without difficulty. But they didn’t.
So, it is not only stripes that deter tabanid
horseflies, other patterns can be effective too. As the “aperture effect” would
not apply to the checked pattern, the researchers could discount it as the
mechanism behind fly confusion.
Professor Caro, the paper’s senior co-author, added: “Not
only do these exciting studies bring us closer to understanding one of the
world’s most iconic and photogenic species, they will be of great interest to
farmers attempting to reduce the damage caused by fly bites and even general
horse-wear companies.”
For more details, see:
Zebra stripes, tabanid biting flies and the aperture effect
Martin J. How, Dunia Gonzales, Alison Irwin and Tim Caro
Proceedings of the Royal Society B Volume 287 Issue 1933
https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.1521
No comments:
Post a Comment